I found some notes on a conversation I had with Griffin last fall. I do not remember the context for it.
Me: Do you know what 12÷2 is?
Griffin (8 years old): 6
Me: How do you know that’s right?
G: 2 times 6 is 12.
Me: What about 26÷2?
G: 13
Me: How do you know that?
G: There were 26 kids in Ms. Starr’s class [in first grade], so it was her magic number. We had 13 pairs of kids.
Me: What about 34÷2?
G: Well, 15 plus 15 is 30…so…19
Here we see the role of cognitive load on mental computation. Griffin is splitting up 34 as 30 and 4 and finding pairs to add to each. Formally, he’s using the distributive property: .
He wants to choose and
so that
.
But by the time he figures out that , he loses track of the fact that
and just adds 4 to 15.
At least, I consider this to be the most likely explanation of his words.
My notes on the conversation only have (back and forth), which indicates that there was some follow-up discussion in which we located and fixed the error. The details are lost to history.
Our conversation continued.
Me: So 12÷2 is 6 because 2×6 is 12. What is 12÷1?
G: [long pause; much longer than for any of the first three tasks] 12.
Me: How do you know this?
G: Because if you gave 1 person 12 things, they would have all 12.
Let’s pause for a moment.
This is what it means to learn mathematics. Mathematical ideas
have multiple interpretations which people encounter as they live their lives. It is (or should be) a major goal of mathematics instruction to help people reconcile these multiple interpretations.
Griffin has so far relied upon three interpretations of division: (1) A division statement is equivalent to a multiplication statement (the fact family interpretation, which is closely related to thinking of division as the inverse of multiplication), (2) Division tells how many groups of a particular size we can make (Ms. Starr’s class has 13 pairs of students—this is the quotative interpretation of division) and (3) Division tells us how many will be in each of a particular number of same-sized groups (Put 12 things into 1 group, and each group has 12 things).
This wasn’t a lesson on multiplication, so I wasn’t too worried about getting Griffin to reconcile these interpretations. Instead, I was curious which (if any) would survive being pushed further.
Me: What is
?
G: [pause, but not as long as for 12÷1] Two.
Me: How do you know that?
G: Half of 12 is 6, and 12÷6 is 2, so it’s 2.
Me: OK. You know what a half dollar is, right?
G: Yeah. 50 cents.
Me: How many half dollars are in a dollar?
G: Two.
Me: How many half dollars are in 12 dollars?
G: [long thoughtful pause] Twenty-four.
Me: How do you know that?
G: I can’t say.
Me: One more. How many quarters are in 12 dollars?
G: Oh no! [pause] Forty-eight. Because a quarter is half of a half and so there are twice as many of them as half dollars. 2 times 24=48.
Another installment in your “math with kids” series — loved it. Will be nice to use with my math for elem students this Fall too.
A very good scenario to teach fractions.
I have also blogged about fractions.
http://www.cbsetuts.com/number-system/fractions/
Let me know your thoughts.
Supports the discussion we are having on decimals and the splitting issue. I want to know more!